全文获取类型
收费全文 | 453篇 |
免费 | 3篇 |
学科分类
工业技术 | 456篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 2篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2020年 | 4篇 |
2019年 | 4篇 |
2018年 | 5篇 |
2017年 | 5篇 |
2016年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 6篇 |
2013年 | 25篇 |
2012年 | 12篇 |
2011年 | 26篇 |
2010年 | 30篇 |
2009年 | 19篇 |
2008年 | 14篇 |
2007年 | 20篇 |
2006年 | 19篇 |
2005年 | 17篇 |
2004年 | 12篇 |
2003年 | 12篇 |
2002年 | 9篇 |
2001年 | 12篇 |
2000年 | 17篇 |
1999年 | 10篇 |
1998年 | 15篇 |
1997年 | 13篇 |
1996年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 17篇 |
1994年 | 19篇 |
1993年 | 8篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 6篇 |
1989年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 11篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 8篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 1篇 |
1966年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有456条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
A comprehensive model of the work–family interface was developed and tested. The proposed model extended prior research by explicitly distinguishing between work interfering with family and family interfering with work. This distinction allowed testing of hypotheses concerning the unique antecedents and outcomes of both forms of work–family conflict and a reciprocal relationship between them. The influence of gender, race, and job type on the generalizability of the model was also examined. Data were obtained through household interviews with a random sample of 631 individuals. The model was tested with structural equation modeling techniques. Results were strongly supportive. In addition, although the model was invariant across gender and race, there were differences across blue- and white-collar workers. Implications for future research on the work–family interface are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
92.
How do people know whether they have an answer to a question before they actually find it in their memory? Two experiments explored this question. Ss were trained on relatively novel 2-digit?×?2-digit arithmetic problems (e.g., 23?×?27). Before answering each problem, Ss made a quick feeling of knowing judgment as to whether they could directly retrieve the answer from memory or had to compute it. Knowing the answer initially appeared to be linearly related to having a feeling of knowing the answer; however, when the frequency of exposure to complete problems and the frequency of exposure to parts of the problems were separately varied, feeling of knowing was better predicted by the frequency of presentation of the problem parts, not by knowledge of the answer. This suggests that the processes involved in knowing the answer are different from those involved in having a feeling of knowing. Specifically, an early feeling of knowing is not just based on an early read of the answer. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
93.
Cox Dena S.; Cox Anthony D.; Sturm Lynne; Zimet Greg 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》2010,29(1):29
Objective: To determine the most effective ways to present human papillomavirus (HPV) vaccine risk and benefit information to mothers in Hispanic, African American, and White communities, to increase mothers’ intentions to vaccinate their daughters against HPV. Design: The study used a 3 × 2 between-subjects factorial design, involving 3 different risk presentation formats (graphical HPV statistics, nongraphical HPV statistics, or no-statistics control) and the presence or absence of rhetorical questions (RQ). Data were collected from a national sample of 471 mothers of girls ages 11–16. Main Outcome Variables: The primary outcome variable was mothers’ intention to vaccinate their daughters against HPV. Secondary outcomes included mothers’ self-reported message comprehension and perceptions of daughters’ vulnerability to HPV infection, infection severity, vaccine efficacy, and obstacles to immunization. Results and Conclusion: Results showed that both risk presentation format and RQs had an overall positive effect on mothers’ intention to vaccinate their daughters. However, the interventions appear to be more effective when used separately than when used in combination. Each of these interventions is brief and could easily be implemented by health care providers as well as in patient health communication literature. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
94.
Horowitz Irwin A.; Bordens Kenneth S.; Victor Elizabeth; Bourgeois Martin J.; ForsterLee Lynne 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》2001,86(4):641
One hundred twenty mock jurors heard 1 of several versions of a civil trial. The tort trial was either high or low in information load and contained evidence that either clearly favored the plaintiffs or was ambiguous. Expert witnesses testified in either technical or less technical language. Verdicts favored the plaintiffs when the evidence was clear and was presented in technical language because technical language enhanced witnesses' credibility when the evidence was clear. Although high information loads and technical language hindered evidence processing, jurors endeavored to comprehend, as indicated by the recall of more facts and alternative constructions of the evidence when that evidence was ambiguous. However, those constructions were of poorer quality, incorporating evidence of lesser probative value. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
95.
The effect of context on responses to questions has been a prominent focus in social and political survey research. However, little investigation of context effects has been done for the measurement of psychological constructs. A measure of anger experience and expression in development uses vignettes describing interpersonal situations that provoke varying degrees of anger and require respondents to indicate their affective and/or expressive response to the situation. In this study, the consequences of pairing the two questions for each vignette are investigated. Pairing the anger-experience and likelihood-of-expression questions changes the item's context. Item response theory analysis similar to that used to detect differential item functioning was performed. For some of the items, responding to a single or paired question affected the extremity of responses. The findings are discussed in terms of their implications for personality measurement. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
96.
Frank A. Hoeberichts Jorge Perez‐Valle Consuelo Montesinos Jos M. Mulet María D. Planes Guillem Hueso Lynne Yenush Sukesh C. Sharma Ramn Serrano 《Yeast (Chichester, England)》2010,27(9):713-725
Glucose, in the absence of additional nutrients, induces programmed cell death in yeast. This phenomenon is independent of yeast metacaspase (Mca1/Yca1) and of calcineurin, requires ROS production and it is concomitant with loss of cellular K+ and vacuolar collapse. K+ is a key nutrient protecting the cells and this effect depends on the Trk1 uptake system and is associated with reduced ROS production. Mutants with decreased activity of plasma membrane H+‐ATPase are more tolerant to glucose‐induced cell death and exhibit less ROS production. A triple mutant ena1‐4 tok1 nha1, devoid of K+ efflux systems, is more tolerant to both glucose‐ and H2O2‐induced cell death. We hypothesize that ROS production, activated by glucose and H+‐ATPase and inhibited by K+ uptake, triggers leakage of K+, a process favoured by K+ efflux systems. Loss of cytosolic K+ probably causes osmotic lysis of vacuoles. The nature of the ROS‐producing system sensitive to K+ and H+ transport is unknown. Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
97.
Osato Sheryl S.; Van Gorp Wilfred G.; Kern Robert S.; Satz Paul; Steinman Lynne 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》1989,1(4):339
Full administration of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised (WAIS—R) was compared to a rescored Satz-Mogel short form of the WAIS—R (P. Satz and S. Mogel, 1962) in 51 elderly, demented individuals. The comparison revealed a high degree of association between these two forms. A smaller subset of 15 Ss was administered the Satz-Mogel short form and the full WAIS—R. Correlations between the two forms were significant for approximately half of the subtests and all of the subscales. No significant differences in means were found between forms. These findings are considered to represent some initial, positive support for the use of the Satz-Mogel short form of the WAIS—R in an elderly, demented population. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
98.
Atwell John L.; Breheney Kerry A.; Lawrence Lynne J.; McCoy Airlie J.; Kortt Alexander A.; Hudson Peter J. 《Protein engineering, design & selection : PEDS》1999,12(7):597-604
Single-chain Fv antibody fragments (scFvs) incorporate a polypeptidelinker to tether the VH and VL domains together. An scFv moleculewith a linker 512 residues long cannot fold into a functionalFv domain and instead associates with a second scFv moleculeto form a bivalent dimer (diabody). Direct ligation of VH andVL domains further restricts association and forces three scFvmolecules to associate into a trivalent trimer (triabody). Wehave defined the effect of linker length on scFv associationby constructing a series of scFvs from anti-neuraminidase antibodyNC10 in which the linker varied from one to four glycine residues.NC10 scFv molecules containing linkers of three and four residuesshowed a strong preference for dimer formation (diabodies),whereas a linker length of one or two glycine residues preventedthe formation of diabodies and directed scFv association intotrimers (triabodies). The data suggest a relatively strict transitionfrom dimer (diabody) to trimer (triabody) upon reduction ofthe linker length from three to two glycine residues. Modellingstudies are consistent with three residues as the minimum linkerlength compatible with diabody formation. Electron microscopeimages of complexes formed between the NC10 scFv multimers andan anti-idiotype Fab' showed that the dimer was bivalent forantigen binding and the trimer was trivalent. 相似文献
99.
Dolezal Olan; Pearce Lesley A.; Lawrence Lynne J.; McCoy Airlie J.; Hudson Peter J.; Kortt Alexander A. 《Protein engineering, design & selection : PEDS》2000,13(8):565-574
Synthetic genes encoding single-chain variable fragments (scFvs)of NC10 anti-neuraminidase antibody were constructed by joiningthe VL and VH domains with linkers of fifteen, five, four, three,two, one and zero residues. These VLVH constructs wereexpressed in Escherichia coli and the resulting proteins werecharacterized and compared with the previously characterizedNC10 scFv proteins assembled in VHVL orientation. Size-exclusionchromatography and electron microscope images of complexes formedbetween various NC10 scFvs and anti-idiotype Fab' were usedto analyse the oligomeric status of these scFvs. The resultshowed that as the linker length between VL and VH was reduced,different patterns of oligomerization were observed comparedwith those with VHVL isomers. As was the case for VHVLorientation, the scFv-15 VLVH protein existed mainlyas a monomer whereas dimer (diabody) was a predominant conformationfor the scFv-5, scFv-4 and scFv-3 VLVH proteins. In contrastto the VHVL isomer, direct ligation of VL to VH led tothe formation of predominantly a tetramer (tetrabody) ratherthan to an expected trimer (triabody). Furthermore, the transitionbetween dimers and higher order oligomers was not as distinctas for VHVL. Thus reducing the linker length in VLVHfrom three to two residues did not precisely dictate a transitionbetween dimers and tetramers. Instead, two-residue as well asone-residue linked scFvs formed a mixture of dimers, trimersand tetramers. 相似文献
100.