首页 | 官方网站   微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2094篇
  免费   110篇
  国内免费   2篇
工业技术   2206篇
  2023年   24篇
  2022年   19篇
  2021年   103篇
  2020年   39篇
  2019年   45篇
  2018年   58篇
  2017年   60篇
  2016年   61篇
  2015年   51篇
  2014年   87篇
  2013年   125篇
  2012年   116篇
  2011年   198篇
  2010年   111篇
  2009年   107篇
  2008年   118篇
  2007年   110篇
  2006年   73篇
  2005年   61篇
  2004年   44篇
  2003年   36篇
  2002年   25篇
  2001年   40篇
  2000年   15篇
  1999年   28篇
  1998年   72篇
  1997年   48篇
  1996年   34篇
  1995年   26篇
  1994年   22篇
  1993年   23篇
  1992年   13篇
  1991年   14篇
  1990年   19篇
  1989年   12篇
  1988年   12篇
  1987年   6篇
  1986年   10篇
  1985年   11篇
  1984年   5篇
  1983年   8篇
  1982年   9篇
  1980年   5篇
  1978年   5篇
  1977年   8篇
  1976年   21篇
  1975年   5篇
  1974年   7篇
  1973年   10篇
  1964年   4篇
排序方式: 共有2206条查询结果,搜索用时 109 毫秒
81.
82.
A Green Revolution for Africa is emerging after decades of neglect of Africa’s agricultural systems. To counter these years of neglect, the then United Nations Secretary-General Kofi Annan called for “a uniquely African Green Revolution”. Since then, a number of initiatives have emerged or are emerging to realize this important vision. As more money and attention galvanizes much-needed action on the African Green Revolution, a vigorous debate is required to ensure that the mission of improving food security on the world’s poorest continent is achieved in the most effective, comprehensive and inclusive manner possible. The African Green Revolution cannot be limited to increasing yields of staple crops but must be designed as a driver of sustainable development, which includes gender empowerment and nutrition elements. This paper first reviews the Asian Green Revolution’s successes and shortcomings from a nutrition and gender perspective and then outlines what the global community can do to ensure that some of the limitations of the Asian Green Revolution, specifically with regard to nutrition and gender, are not repeated.  相似文献   
83.
Populations of native unionids have been in steady decline over the past century. The invasion of dreissenid mussels (Dreissena polymorpha and Dreissena rostriformis bugensis) in the mid-1980's impacted already imperiled unionid populations by greatly increasing their regional extinction rates. A selection of Great Lakes coastal wetlands around Michigan was surveyed to locate remnant populations of native unionids. Physical and chemical parameters were measured in coastal wetlands to evaluate the importance of these habitat parameters to remnant unionid assemblages. We assessed fouling rates by dreissenids on unionids and used artificial substrates to estimate dreissenid colonization densities. Live unionids were found in coastal wetlands of the Les Cheneaux Islands, the Lake St. Clair delta, and North Maumee Bay with significantly higher unionid fouling in the Les Cheneaux Islands compared to the other two sampling areas (F2,76 = 4.97, p = 0.0095). No live unionids were documented in Beaver Island, Garden Island, Grand Traverse Bay, or Saginaw Bay wetlands. Dreissena colonization densities on artificial substrates averaged 19,213 m− 2 at one site in North Maumee Bay, and 10,425 m− 2 in Saginaw Bay, but no colonization occurred in the wetlands of Beaver Island, Garden Island, the Les Cheneaux Islands, or Grand Traverse Bay while Dreissena presence in the open water of these regions was evident. Dreissena colonization densities on artificial substrates increased with measures of anthropogenic disturbance and decreased with higher water level fluctuations and aerial exposure. Specific conductance, turbidity, and magnitude of water level fluctuations were important predictors of Dreissena colonization on artificial substrates.  相似文献   
84.
Professional psychology has increasingly moved toward evidence-based practice. However, instruments used to assess psychologists seeking licensure, such as the Examination for Professional Practice in Psychology (EPPP), have received relatively little empirical scrutiny. Therefore, the authors evaluated the available evidence in support of the EPPP’s validity and current use as a core component of professional licensure. Although the EPPP has in many ways been extensively evaluated, there is a paucity of criterion, predictive, and incremental validity evidence available. Further, several aspects of the content validation studies were examined, and the authors question whether the EPPP, as currently constructed, can meet its stated goals. Given that the EPPP is a high-stakes examination and given the authors’ best estimate (based on a sample of 16 states) that 35% of applicants fail the examination, it is recommended that the EPPP be more extensively evaluated. An outline of major decision points in this proposed evaluation process is provided, several suggestions for further research are proposed, and the field is encouraged to discuss these issues further. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
85.
The authors show that a narrowband noise (NBN) is perceived as longer when presented immediately after a wideband noise (WBN), compared to when the WBN is absent. This effect depended on the WBN’s frequency spectrum overlapping that of the NBN, and it increased as the duration of the WBN increased up to 300 ms. It decreased when a silent gap was introduced between the WBN and NBN, but remained significant for an easily detectable gap of 40 ms. A correlate of the effect was observed in the mismatch negativity (MMN) to a deviant stimulus, consisting of a WBN + NBN, presented in a sequence of more common isolated WBNs. The MMN latency was longer for an on-frequency than for an off-frequency WBN; and, more importantly, the size of this difference correlated across participants with the difference in perceived duration. A rhythm-adjustment experiment showed that the presence of an on-frequency WBN immediately preceding a tone caused that tone to be heard as starting earlier than when the WBN was absent. The results are discussed in relation to the continuity illusion and models of duration encoding. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
86.
In 2 studies, the authors developed and validated of a new set of standardized emotion expressions, which they referred to as the University of California, Davis, Set of Emotion Expressions (UCDSEE). The precise components of each expression were verified using the Facial Action Coding System (FACS). The UCDSEE is the first FACS-verified set to include the three “self-conscious” emotions known to have recognizable expressions (embarrassment, pride, and shame), as well as the 6 previously established “basic” emotions (anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, and surprise), all posed by the same 4 expressers (African and White males and females). This new set has numerous potential applications in future research on emotion and related topics. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
87.
Creativity is commonly defined as the novel and useful combination of concepts previously thought to be unrelated. Fundamentally, this process rests on the ability to judge relatedness between concepts. The authors conducted an experiment to test the connection between divergent thinking ability and the speed of judging relatedness. Our results demonstrated that people with higher divergent thinking ability are faster in judging whether two concepts are related or unrelated. In real life, this can lead to a substantial advantage in the number of potentially useful relationships that could be assessed per unit of time. Equally important, there was no link between IQ and speed of judging relatedness. These results are interpreted in relation to a recently proposed model of creative cognition suggesting that creative people are faster in information processing under conditions of low ambiguity. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
88.
The present research surveyed a group of editors and editorial board members of personality and social psychology journals to examine the practice of psychological science in their field. Findings demonstrate that (a) although personality and social researchers tend to use many of the same approaches, methods, and procedures, they nonetheless show average differences in each of these domains, as well as in their overarching theoretical aims and perspectives; (b) these average differences largely conform to social and personality researchers’ stereotypes about each subgroup; (c) despite their methodological and philosophical differences, the 2 subgroups study many of the same research topics; and (d) the structure of social–personality research practices can be characterized as having 2 independent factors, which closely correspond to L. J. Cronbach’s (1957) correlational and experimental “streams of research.” (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
89.
There is increasing interest among architectural historians in addressing environmental concerns on both historical and theoretical terms. Simultaneously, other fields have been looking to architectural scholarship to understand the historical relationship between the built and the natural environment. For architectural historians, and others, this has also involved correlating the shifting discourse on environment with a history of architectural transformations and disciplinary expansions. These engagements have made clear that the environmental history of architecture does not simply add more objects to the historical database, but also changes the terms of historical analysis, as new matters of concern and new conceptual frameworks come to the fore. This paper gathers together a dialogic set of projections from scholars responding to the question of how we might newly understand the historical relationship between the built and the natural environment, and the opportunities and challenges this new phase presents to scholars, design researchers, and architects.  相似文献   
90.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司    京ICP备09084417号-23

京公网安备 11010802026262号