首页 | 官方网站   微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   963篇
  免费   27篇
工业技术   990篇
  2023年   2篇
  2022年   9篇
  2021年   10篇
  2020年   10篇
  2019年   5篇
  2018年   8篇
  2017年   15篇
  2016年   20篇
  2015年   10篇
  2014年   20篇
  2013年   51篇
  2012年   36篇
  2011年   45篇
  2010年   46篇
  2009年   43篇
  2008年   55篇
  2007年   45篇
  2006年   35篇
  2005年   37篇
  2004年   31篇
  2003年   26篇
  2002年   42篇
  2001年   21篇
  2000年   13篇
  1999年   17篇
  1998年   21篇
  1997年   23篇
  1996年   24篇
  1995年   25篇
  1994年   10篇
  1993年   28篇
  1992年   23篇
  1991年   7篇
  1990年   21篇
  1989年   19篇
  1988年   12篇
  1987年   12篇
  1986年   18篇
  1985年   14篇
  1984年   12篇
  1983年   10篇
  1982年   7篇
  1981年   3篇
  1980年   5篇
  1979年   12篇
  1978年   8篇
  1977年   8篇
  1976年   5篇
  1975年   3篇
  1973年   4篇
排序方式: 共有990条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
141.
Evaluated 5 causal models of educational productivity as they apply to learning science among 882 5th–8th graders. Each model explores the relationship between achievement, as assessed by the Comprehensive Test of Basic Skills, and a combination of 8 constructs: home environment, peer group, media, ability, social environment, time on task, motivation, and instructional strategies. Linear structural-relations analysis was employed to examine each of the models. The complex theoretical model had significant (positive) coefficients for all the constructs except quality of instruction; goodness-of-fit indices showed it was the best fitting model with the lowest average residual size. (13 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
142.
Investigated the effects of response cues (erotic, romantic, or neutral) and level of sex guilt on the self-reported sexual fantasies of 90 undergraduate females. Ss completed a sex guilt inventory (Masher Forced-Choice Guilt Inventory—Female Form), the Nowlis Mood Adjective Check List, the Fantasy Theme Checklist, and ratings of their affective responses and physiological arousal associated with the writing of the fantasies. High sex guilt Ss preferred fantasy themes indicating a lack of responsibility for engaging in sexual interaction. Ss in the erotic fantasy condition wrote more explicit fantasies and described more varied content. Arousal seemed to be affected by the response cuing in the predicted direction but not by the Ss' guilt levels. Sex guilt level seemed to be a better predictor of affective responses, such as guilt and embarrassment, than the response cuing. Results suggest that sexual fantasy behavior may be part of a cluster of sexual behaviors governed by an individual's level of sex guilt. It is suggested that the demonstration that fantasy production seemed to be influenced by situational demands has implications for collection and use of fantasy information by both clinicians and researchers. (29 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
143.
Thin sections of nervous tissue were viewed at different tilt angles using a transmission electron microscope equipped with a eucentric goniometer stage. In a comparison study of various degrees of tilt, one can observe additional morphological features within synaptic profiles, define subsynaptic structures such as Taxi-bodies, and clearly see the crystalline formation of cytochemical tracers. This study demonstrates the value of tilting thin-sections in the analysis of synapses and other biological material at the ultrastructural level.  相似文献   
144.
The self-definitional processes accompanying the transition to motherhood were examined in this study. A cross-sectional sample of more than 600 women who were planning to get pregnant within 2 years, pregnant, or in the postpartum stage completed extensive questionnaires pertaining to their experiences of pregnancy and motherhood. On the basis of the assumption of the "self-socialization" perspective that individuals actively construct their identities in response to life transitions, our analyses focused on the role of information-seeking in the developing self-definitions of women becoming mothers. As predicted, (a) women actively sought information in anticipation of a first birth, (b) they used this information to construct identities incorporating motherhood, and (c) after the birth the determinants of their self-definitions shifted from indirect sources of information to direct experiences with child care. Hence, consistent with the self-socialization perspective, information-seeking did play an important role in the women's developing self-conceptions during this life transition. Mechanisms by which information gathered may alter self-conception are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
145.
In order to track neuropsychological recovery as a function of age more precisely than in prior studies, 31 alcoholics were divided into 3 age groups (20–29, 30–39, and ≥ 40 yrs) and administered a repeatable neuropsychological test battery for the 3 mo immediately following cessation of drinking. Two control groups (15 college students and 15 individuals ≥ age 40 yrs) were also tested to assess practice effects, optimal test performance, and age-related test performance. Prior research has shown the tests used are sensitive to alcohol-related dysfunction and other transient drug toxicity. A variety of alcohol consumption and demographic variables were also studied as predictors of performance deficits. Results show that (a) alcoholics below age 40 yrs recovered completely from functional impairment by 2–3 wks after drinking ceased, and (b) alcoholics aged ≥ 40 yrs failed to recover over 3 mo on some but not all visuo-spatial tasks. Results suggest that beyond 40 yrs, humans increase in vulnerability to the toxic effects of alcohol or are less able to compensate for impairment of the neurological substrate. (18 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
146.
Investigated preschoolers' knowledge of counting principles by examining their ability to discriminate between features that are essential for correct counting and features that are typically present but unessential. The standard counting procedure was analyzed into 1 essential feature, word/object correspondence (WOC), and 4 optional features: counting adjacent objects consecutively, pointing once to each object, starting at an end of a row, and proceeding in a left to right direction. In Exp I, 10 3-, 10 4-, and 10 5-yr-olds were asked to judge a puppet's counting that either violated the essential or unessential features or that conformed to the standard correct procedure. Ss who knew the WOC principle presumably would reject counts that violated it more often than counts that conformed to it. Each S's skill in counting rows of objects also was assessed. In Exp II, 16 3-yr-olds completed a similar task but were able to see an adult model perform the task before judging. Skill in executing the standard counting procedure preceded knowledge of the underlying principle. Four- and 5-yr-olds knew that WOC was essential, although a high percentage of them did not know that other typical features were unessential. An analysis of probable environmental input and of the features' utility in separating already-counted from to-be-counted objects is proposed to account for the relative probabilities that Ss knew that each of the 5 features of standard counting was essential or optional. (16 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
147.
The aim of this study was to seek evidence of intermodal knowledge about gender in infants that would provide direct evidence of the existence of gender categories during the 1st yr of life. In Exp 1, 20 9- and 24 12-mo-olds were presented with pairs of male and female pictures with a female or male voice presented simultaneously. Ss spent significantly more time looking at the pictures matching the voices than at the same pictures paired with mismatching voices, but only in the case of female stimuli. Comparison to chance level performance suggested that the matching effect was more consistent in older Ss. In Exp 2, 20 9-mo-olds were tested with a set of highly stereotypical faces and distinctive male and female voices. Ss showed a preference for the faces matching the voices, but this effect was again restricted to female stimuli. Results of both studies suggest that intermodal knowledge about gender develops during the 2nd half of the 1st yr. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
148.
We investigated the impact of three variables on students' ratings of instruction (SRIs): social contact between instructor and students (present vs. absent), the instructor's facial expression (smiling vs. neutral), and the instructor's sex. Subjects were presented with hypothetical scenarios in which these factors varied while behaviors directly related to teaching were held constant. Results revealed an interaction between instructor sex and both of the other two factors: Behaviors indicative of friendliness toward students elevated SRIs for female instructors but not for male instructors. In addition, subjects rated the male professors as more effective than female professors. These findings are consistent with other reports that students expect female instructors to excel in both stereotypically masculine (e.g., competence) and feminine (e.g., warmth) domains. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
149.
Let X be a random variable such that In [(X - ?)/?] has a s-normal distribution with mean zero and variance one. Then X has a 3-parameter lognormal distribution with the third parameter, the shape parameter, fixed at unity. This paper presents the coefficients required to construct the best linear unbiased estimators (BLUEs) of ? and ? for samples of size fifteen and less. The variances and covariances of these estimators are provided. These estimators yield the BLUEs of the mean, standard deviation, and percentiles of X since these quantities are linear functions of ? and ?. Blom's estimators and maximum likelihood estimators compare favorably with the BLUEs.  相似文献   
150.
In 3 experiments, 150 young adults saw black-and-white line drawings and imagined them filled in with a specified color. A surprise recognition test followed, with the drawings shown colored in. Drawings shown in the color in which they had been imagined were more likely to be identified than those shown in a different color. (French abstract) (11 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司    京ICP备09084417号-23

京公网安备 11010802026262号