共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 95 毫秒
1.
视觉形象经常被分为结构上的层级图形 ,脑对层级图形的视觉认知加工机制近年来得到细致的研究。韩世辉等发现正常人图形加工为整体优势 ,帕金森病 (PD)患者的视觉整体与局部的认知功能状况尚未见报道。我们对PD患者进行研究 ,以了解PD患者视觉整体与局部认知功能状况。资料和方法 :选门诊PD患者 30例及正常对照 30名 (NC组 ) ,利用改良Webster评分、Hoehn Yahr分级判断PD患者运动障碍程度及疾病分期 ;利用中国科技大学研究生院设计的视觉整体与局部认知功能检查软件对患者进行检查。刺激图是由小箭头组成的大箭… 相似文献
2.
目的 探讨帕金森病(PD)患者视觉整体与局部认知功能。方法 利用中国科技大学研究生院认知实验室设计的整体与局部认知功能检查软件对30例PD患者和31例对照进行检查。结果 PD患者的反应时间(RT)较对照组无显著性差异(P>0.05)。进一步研究发现左侧肢体起病PD患者RT较对照组延长,右侧起病PD患者RT较对照组缩短,但除左侧起病PD患者整体(大小箭头方向一致)情况外,均无显著性差异。将左侧和右侧起病的PD患者进行对比发现,左侧起病患者各项RT(除局部大小箭头方向不一致情况外)均较右侧起病患者显著延长(P<0.05)。结论右侧基底节参与了视觉整体与局部的认知加工,左侧基底节可能对右侧基底节有抑制性调节作用。 相似文献
3.
正常人的视觉事件相关电位研究 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
本文利用三种视觉符号编排的刺激序列,记录了95名正常人的视觉ERP,报道了Fz、Pz点N2、P3波幅和潜伏期的正常值及N2、P3的影响因素。结果表明:该刺激序列可引出清晰、稳定的ERP波形;P3波幅与任务和部位有关,靶P3波幅大于非靶P3,靶P3于Pz点波幅大于Fz,非靶P3在Fz、Pz点间波幅无明显差异;N2波幅与任务无关,靶与非靶之间N2波幅地无差异,而与部位有关,Fz点波大于Pz点;N2、P 相似文献
4.
1例顶叶萎缩患者的神经心理学研究 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
双侧顶叶局限性进行性萎缩非常少见[1],我们发现1例,报道如下。1资料1.1一般资料:患者,女,67岁,右利手,文化程度大专,退休前是中学老师,因反应迟钝、主动语言减少2年于2005年11月就诊本院神经内科,头颅MR显示:双侧顶叶萎缩(图1)。大脑SPECT检查呈现顶叶局部脑血流量降低。MRA没 相似文献
5.
6.
关陆阳;侯文龙;曹佳琪;庄楠楠;陈茹凤;殷旭圆;朱振华;王菁;贾秋放;惠李;常静 《中华精神科杂志》2021,54(03):177-183
目的 研究首次发病未服药精神分裂症患者的立体视觉及其与认知功能的关系。 方法 本研究起止时间为2019年1月至2020年9月;共招募146例首次发病未服药精神分裂症患者(首发精分组)、124例慢性期精神分裂症患者(慢性精分组)和101名健康对照者(对照组)。采用Titmus方法检测立体视觉,阳性与阴性症状量表评定患者临床症状,重复性成套神经心理状态测验(Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status,RBANS)评估认知功能。采用协方差分析3组间立体视觉和认知功能的差异,Spearman相关和多元线性回归分析立体视觉和临床症状、认知功能的相关性。 结果 (1)3组间的立体视觉水平和认知功能方面的差异有统计学意义(P<0.01),事后比较分析发现首发精分组、慢性精分组的立体视觉和认知水平分别与对照组差异有统计学意义,而首发精分组和慢性精分组的差异无统计学意义。(2)Spearman相关分析显示,首发精分组的立体视觉水平与临床症状严重程度的相关不显著,但与视觉广度(r=-0.193, P=0.019)、言语功能(r=-0.261, P=0.001)、注意功能(r=-0.168, P=0.042)和RBANS总分(r=-0.236, P=0.004)显著相关。对照组和慢性精分组的立体视觉水平与临床症状严重程度、认知功能相关均不显著。进一步多元线性回归发现,立体视觉对首发精分组的视觉广度(β=-0.213, P=0.011)、言语功能(β=-0.252, P=0.003)、注意功能(β=-0.189, P=0.019)和RBANS总分(β=-0.235, P=0.003)仍有显著的影响。 结论 首发精分组和慢性精分组均存在显著的立体视觉和认知功能障碍;首发精分组立体视觉的水平与临床症状严重程度相关不显著,但与认知功能相关。 相似文献
7.
目的:探讨多发性硬化(MS)患者事件相关电位(ERP)和视觉诱发电位(VEP)的变化.方法:对40例复发缓解型MS患者和40例健康人的ERP和VEP进行分析对比.结果:复发缓解型MS患者ERP的N2、P3波的潜伏期较对照组延长(P<0.05),VEP的P100、N145波的潜伏期较对照组延长(P<0.05).结论:ER... 相似文献
8.
9.
邬岳清 《国际精神病学杂志》2021,48(4):684-686
目的分析和探讨脑小血管病认知功能障碍与脑电的相关性。方法以2017年9月~2019年9月为时间基准,在我院所有脑小血管病认知功能障碍患者当中,随机收集60例作为本次研究的对象,将其设定为观察组,同期选取60名健康人群,作为对照组。对两组人群均进行脑电图检测,对比两组脑电图检测结果以及精神状态量表评分和蒙特利尔认知评估量表评分。结果观察组脑电图的正常比例低于对照组,边缘、轻度异常以及中重度异常比例均高于对照组,观察组治疗前后脑电图结果中正常、中轻度异常项目中,治疗前后有差异性(F=3.245、2.321,P=0.000,0.000),观察组的记忆力评分、定向力评分、计算力评分、注意力评分、语言能力评分以及总评分均低于对照组,观察组的执行能力评分、命名能力评分、注意力评分、计算力评分、语言能力评分、抽象思维能力评分、延迟回忆力评分、定向力评分以及总评分评分均高于对照组(P<0.05)。结论脑电图可以对脑小血管病认知功能障碍进行诊断,也可以作为评估其治疗效果的工具,适合在临床进行开展和普及。 相似文献
10.
11.
Newport R Brown L Husain M Mort D Jackson SR 《Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior》2006,42(5):720-729
We studied a patient (J.J.) with bilateral damage to those regions of the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) thought to be involved in prism adaptation. We demonstrated, for the first time in a parietal patient, that J.J. was unable to adapt to the visual perturbation induced by the optical prisms with either hand within four times the number of trials required by healthy adult subjects. We offer a novel account for the role of the PPC in prism adaptation: that the reach direction to the veridical target location specified in extrinsic limb-based coordinates must be de-coupled from the gaze direction to the perceived target location specified by intrinsic oculocentric coordinates in order to produce spatially accurate movements. This spatial discrepancy between gaze direction and reach direction may provide the necessary training signal required by the cerebellum to update the current internal model used to maintain spatial congruency between visual and proprioceptive maps of peripersonal space. The hypothesis is discussed in relation to recent disconnectionist accounts of optic ataxia. 相似文献
12.
13.
Pegah Sarkheil Rainer Goebel Frank Schneider Klaus Mathiak 《Social cognitive and affective neuroscience》2013,8(8):950-957
Facial expressions convey important emotional and social information and are frequently applied in investigations of human affective processing. Dynamic faces may provide higher ecological validity to examine perceptual and cognitive processing of facial expressions. Higher order processing of emotional faces was addressed by varying the task and virtual face models systematically. Blood oxygenation level-dependent activation was assessed using functional magnetic resonance imaging in 20 healthy volunteers while viewing and evaluating either emotion or gender intensity of dynamic face stimuli. A general linear model analysis revealed that high valence activated a network of motion-responsive areas, indicating that visual motion areas support perceptual coding for the motion-based intensity of facial expressions. The comparison of emotion with gender discrimination task revealed increased activation of inferior parietal lobule, which highlights the involvement of parietal areas in processing of high level features of faces. Dynamic emotional stimuli may help to emphasize functions of the hypothesized ‘extended’ over the ‘core’ system for face processing. 相似文献
14.
Impaired ability to draw visually presented figures by copying represents one major manifestation of constructional apraxia (CA). Previous clinical studies have indicated that CA is caused by lesions in the posterior parietal cortex (PPC), but the functional roles of the PPC remain unclear. A spared ability to trace with an impaired ability to copy indicates that deficits lie not in low-level visuomotor processing, but rather in a coordinate transformation involving production of an egocentric representation of model trajectory in the drawing space, which is spatially separated from the model space. To test the hypothesis that the PPC plays a role in coordinate transformation, we compared brain activities for drawing by copying and tracing using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Healthy participants traced over the visually presented model or copied the model on a separate space. To avoid potential confounders of differences in behavioral performances as well as eye movements, a memory-guided condition was introduced, resulting in four drawing conditions; tracing over or copying a model at different locations (tracing and copying), with or without an on-screen model (visual and memory guidance). As hypothesized, the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) bilaterally in the PPC showed significantly greater activations in copying than in tracing, under both visual and memory guidance, with a distinct activation pattern involving the premotor and mesial motor regions. This study indicates a role of the PPC in coordinate transformation for drawing by copying, which may be important for the copying deficit observed in CA. 相似文献
15.
16.
Present evidence suggests that medial temporal cortices subserve allocentric representation and memory, whereas egocentric representation and memory mainly depends on inferior and superior parietal cortices. Virtual reality environments have a major advantage for the assessment of spatial navigation and memory formation, as computer-simulated first-person environments can simulate navigation in a large-scale space. However, virtual reality studies on allocentric memory in subjects with cortical lesions are rare, and studies on egocentric memory are lacking. Twenty-four subjects with unilateral parietal cortex lesions due to infarction or intracerebral haemorrhage (14 left-sided, 10 right-sided) were compared with 36 healthy matched control subjects on two virtual reality tasks affording to learn a virtual park (allocentric memory) and a virtual maze (egocentric memory). Subjects further received a comprehensive clinical and neuropsychological investigation, and MRI lesion assessment using T1, T2 and FLAIR sequences as well as 3D MRI volumetry at the time of the assessment. Results indicate that left- and right-sided lesioned subjects did not differ on task performance. Compared with control subjects, subjects with parietal cortex lesions were strongly impaired learning the virtual maze. On the other hand, performance of subjects with parietal cortex lesions on the virtual park was entirely normal. Volumes of the right-sided precuneus of lesioned subjects were significantly related to performance on the virtual maze, indicating better performance of subjects with larger volumes. It is concluded that parietal cortices support egocentric navigation and imagination during spatial learning in large-scale environments. 相似文献
17.
《Developmental medicine and child neurology》2008,50(3):234-236
Book Reviewed in this articles.
Neurological Assessment in the First Two Years of Life Edited by Giovanni Cioni and Eugenio Mercuri 相似文献
Neurological Assessment in the First Two Years of Life Edited by Giovanni Cioni and Eugenio Mercuri 相似文献
18.
目的 枕叶癫痫的特征表现视觉症状不易被察觉,进行视觉症状分析,提高对枕叶癫痫症状学的认识,减少误诊、漏诊.方法 选择2017年6月至2019年12月就诊于空军军医大学西京医院癫痫专科门诊确诊为枕叶癫痫的患者128例,选取其中有视觉症状表现的59例患者进行视觉症状、病因学、脑电图、治疗及预后分析.结果 59例枕叶癫痫患者... 相似文献
19.
Mazza M Costagliola C Di Michele V Magliani V Pollice R Ricci A Di Giovanbattista E Roncone R Casacchia M Galzio RJ 《European archives of psychiatry and clinical neuroscience》2007,257(1):12-22
The ability of humans to predict and explain other people’s behaviour by attributing independent mental states such as desires
and beliefs to them, is considered to be due to our ability to construct a “Theory of Mind”. Recently, several neuroimaging
studies have implicated the medial frontal lobes as playing a critical role in a dedicated “mentalizing” or “Theory of Mind”
network in the human brain. In this study we compare the performance of patients with right and left medial prefrontal lobe
lesions in theory of mind and in social cognition tasks, with the performance of people with schizophrenia.
We report a similar social cognitive profile between patients with prefrontal lobe lesions and schizophrenic subjects in terms
of understanding of false beliefs, in understanding social situations and in using tactical strategies. These findings are
relevant for the functional anatomy of “Theory of Mind”. 相似文献
20.
In this preliminary study, visual search for targets within and beyond the initial field of view was investigated in seven school-aged children (five females, two males; mean age at testing 8 years 10 months, SD 1 year 3 months; range 6 to 10 years) with various acquired, postnatal, focal brain injuries (haematoma, haemorrhage, meningioma, neuroblastoma, and cerebral abscess) in anterior or posterior sites of the left or right hemisphere, and seven control children (matched for age and sex) were also studied. All participants attended mainstream primary schools. The children with lesions underwent surgery after diagnosis (mean age at diagnosis 5 years 4 months, SD 2 years 7 months). Group results indicated that for the overall scores on three psychometric tests of visuospatial and fine motor abilities (Southern California Figure Ground Perception Test, Visual Organization Test, and Visual-Motor Integration Test), no difference between the children with left and right lesions was present. However, children with lesions in the right hemisphere, and not in the left hemisphere, took significantly more time than the controls to locate visual targets presented within and beyond the field of view. Examination of individual data suggested that, in accordance with brain imaging research, right-sided anterior cerebral lesions sustained in early childhood might have an enduring detrimental effect on voluntary visual search performance during development. This persistent effect of early brain injury might imply that developmental plasticity of the brain does not apply to certain specific functions of particular areas of the right hemisphere. 相似文献