共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 20 毫秒
1.
Uma Ramakrishnan Richard G. Coss Jeffrey Schank Amita Dharawat & Susan Kim 《Ethology : formerly Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie》2005,111(4):337-356
Wild bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata) were studied in southern India to assess their ability to discriminate non‐venomous, venomous and predatory snakes. Realistic snake models were presented to eight troops of bonnet macaques at feeding stations and their behavior was video‐recorded 3 min before and 3 min after snake exposure. Snakes presented were: (1) venomous Indian cobra (Naja naja) displaying an open hood with ‘eyespots’; (2) venomous common Indian krait (Bungarus caeruleus); (3) non‐venomous green keelback (Macropisthodan plumbicolor); (4) non‐venomous rat snake (Ptyas mucosus); and (5) Indian python (Python molurus) which preys on macaques. Latencies to detect and react to the snakes were evaluated to determine initial responsiveness. Longer‐term assessment was measured as the percentage of time individuals looked at the snakes and monitored the activity of nearby individuals before and after snake detection. All snake models engendered caution and maintenance of a safe distance. Alarm calling occurred only during python presentations. The cobra engendered a startle response or running in the largest percentage of individuals after its detection, whereas the rat snake and python elicited bipedal standing or ambulating to monitor the snakes. We also examined the influence of age on snake recognition. Juveniles and subadults looked at the cobra, krait, and python for a larger percentage of time than adults did; albeit, adults looked at the python substantially longer than at the other snakes. Age differences in behavior suggest that, with the exception of the python, repeated experience with snakes in the wild moderates excitability, consistent with the likely threat of envenomation. 相似文献
2.
Anantha Krishna Sharma Mewa Singh Werner Kaumanns Ellen Krebs Mridula Singh M. Ananda Kumar H. N. Kumara 《International journal of primatology》2006,27(5):1429-1439
During a 5-yr study of lion-tailed macaques in their natural habitat, we found that: 1) most births occurred from January to April (70%) and from September to December (19%), showing a bimodal pattern with a major and a minor birth peak; 2) the period of peaks remained the same over 5 yr; and 3) a similar pattern of birth peaks occurred both in groups in large forest complexes that had overlapping home ranges with other groups and in single groups that were isolated in forest fragments in the same region but with similar ecological conditions. The results suggest more of a birth seasonality than mere breeding synchronization in the wild lion-tailed macaques. We also analyzed data on births in captivity in European Zoos for 10 yr. We observed no seasonality or peaks in births, and the pattern was the same over the years. Data on rainfall suggest that resource availability in the wild habitat may not be uniform throughout the year; hence, ecological factors may play an important role in determination of birth patterns in the natural habitats of lion-tailed macaques. 相似文献
3.
4.
5.
Arlet Małgorzata E. Balasubramaniam Krishna N. Saha Rajarshi Beisner Brianne Marty Pascal R. Kaburu Stefano S. K. Bliss-Moreau Eliza Kaasik Ants Kodandaramaiah Ullasa McCowan Brenda 《International journal of primatology》2021,42(2):220-236
International Journal of Primatology - Female reproductive success depends to a large extent on infants’ ability to survive to maturity. While most studies of female reproductive success have... 相似文献
6.
Lactating bonnet monkeys were used as a model to understand the mechanism of ovarian quiescence during lactation. The ovary of the bonnet monkey in the 3rd month of lactation responds well to exogenous pregnant mare serum gonadotropin stimulation with serum estrogen values reaching maximal levels by day 3 of the gonadotropin injection. The adminstration of ovine prolactin to such monkeys significantly inhibited the ovarian responsiveness to exogenous gonadotropin. The responsiveness of the pituitary of the lactating monkey (in the 3rd month of lactation) to luteinizing hormone releasing hormone injection was suppressed and supplementation with exogenous prolactin further accentuating this effect. The relative ability of chlorpromazine given intravenously/intramuscularly/intranasally to enhance endogenous prolactin levels was assessed. During the first 5 months of lactation when the basal prolactin levels were high, the luteinizing hormone levels were low. As the suckling stimulus reduces and prolactin levels fall, luteinizing hormone levels increase, the first post-parturient mensus occurring by 218 ± 4 days. This event was postponed by 3 months on increasing endogenous prolactin levels by administering chlorpromazine (250 μg/day by intranasal mode) over a 5 day period every month starting from the 3rd month of lactation. 相似文献
7.
We studied time budgeting of lion-tailed macaques in the Anamalai Wildlife Sanctuary, Tamil Nadu, India, with regard to seven activity categories. Groups spent >50% of their time feeding and foraging, 33% of the time resting, and 15% moving. More energetic activities were spread uniformly through the day. Feeding had two annual peaks: one closely following the northeast monsoon and the other corresponding to the southwest monsoon. These also coincided with peaks of food availability and mating. Time saved in feeding was spent mostly on resting. Ranging showed a pattern contrary to that of time spent moving and was related more to weather than to food availability; it increased during cooler periods. The middle and upper stories of the canopy were used >93% of the time. The ground was used rarely. Time budgeting characteristics of the lion-tailed macaque are primarily frugiinsectivorous adaptations to a wet evergreen forest habitat. 相似文献
8.
Richard G. Coss Brenda McCowan† & Uma Ramakrishnan‡ 《Ethology : formerly Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie》2007,113(4):352-367
Wild and urban bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata) were studied in southern India to record alarm calls during presentations of realistic models of spotted and dark leopards (Panthera pardus) and an Indian python (Python molurus). Recordings of alarm calls were made from members of four forest troops at feeding stations who observed brief and prolonged presentations of fully exposed spotted and dark leopard morphs and partially concealed views of the spotted morph. Four different forest troops were presented a slowly moving python near feeding stations. Two predator‐inexperienced urban troops from the city of Bangalore were presented either the spotted leopard morph briefly or the python. Analyses of alarm calls revealed differences in acoustic structure, such as a lower harmonic to noise ratio, which can be interpreted as reflecting the level of perceived threat rather than predator type. Noisy alarm calls likely indicate high states of physiological arousal that might provide eavesdropping troop members with information useful for assessing the urgency of the predatory threat. Lack of alarm‐call distinctiveness characterizing predator type is complemented by explicit contextual information in which alarm calling to leopards never occurred on the ground whereas nearly all initial python‐elicited alarm calls were made by individuals on the ground monitoring the python. The alarm calls of Bangalore monkeys distinguished the leopard and python models, with the latter engendering the noisiest calls and immediate flight to trees. Such flight is unnecessary with the python and suggests that, without appropriate experience with pythons, bonnet macaques adopt less predator specific refuge‐seeking behavior. 相似文献
9.
Odile Petit Christine Desportes Bernard Thierry 《Ethology : formerly Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie》1992,90(2):107-120
This study aimed to assess the conditions under which monkeys might produce common effects in an instrumental task. One group of Tonkean macaques (Macaca tonkeana) and two groups of rhesus macaques (M. mulatta) were given series of tests in which food incentives were placed under stones of various weights. In the Tonkean group, the simultaneous action of two subjects in moving the same stone was a relatively frequently occurring event: coaction was more effective than individual action for moving heavy stones; however, there were no signs of coordination among partners and no improvement through learning. In rhesus groups, instances of coaction were extremely rare, and rates of agonistic interactions were higher than in the Tonkean group. It is suggested that “coproduction” represented an emergent phenomenon resulting from social interactions and, as such, was dependent on the level of interindividual tolerance. 相似文献
10.
Basic morphometric data were collected from 22 adult lion-tailed macaques (M. silenus) of both sexes. M. silenus is a rare primate species from which adequate morphometric data have not heretofore been available for comparative purposes. Data collected include measures of gross body size (weight; crown-rump and rump-heel length), and for males, measures of secondary sexual characteristics (canine tooth and testes size). Degree of sexual dimorphism was marked, with males significantly larger and heavier than females. The three body size measures were correlated for males but not for females. There was substantial variation among individual males in secondary sex characteristics measurements. The data indicate than lion-tailed macaque morphometrics are consonant with the general pattern of positive allometry for body size and sexual dimorphism characteristic of the primate order. 相似文献
11.
12.
N E Schaffer M Cranfield A T Fazleabas R S Jeyendran 《Journal of reproduction and fertility》1989,86(2):767-770
The bladder of 6 lion-tailed macaques was emptied and flushed with sterile saline. TALP-Hepes buffer was infused and the animals were electroejaculated. After electroejaculation, the semen quality was determined in the ejaculate and the bladder infusate. Of the 15 ejaculates analysed, a mean (+/- s.e.m.) sperm count of 133.8 (+/- 30.7) x 10(6) with 69.5 (+/- 6.0)% motility was obtained in the infusate as compared to the sperm count of 72.4 (+/- 38.6) x 10(6) with significantly lower (47.7 +/- 5.8%) motility in the ejaculate. 相似文献
13.
Malhi RS Sickler B Lin D Satkoski J Tito RY George D Kanthaswamy S Smith DG 《PloS one》2007,2(5):e438
We developed a novel method for identifying SNPs widely distributed throughout the coding and non-coding regions of a genome. The method uses large-scale parallel pyrosequencing technology in combination with bioinformatics tools. We used this method to generate approximately 23,000 candidate SNPs throughout the Macaca mulatta genome. We estimate that over 60% of the SNPs will be of high frequency and useful for mapping QTLs, genetic management, and studies of individual relatedness, whereas other less frequent SNPs may be useful as population specific markers for ancestry identification. We have created a web resource called MamuSNP to view the SNPs and associated information online. This resource will also be useful for researchers using a wide variety of Macaca species in their research. 相似文献
14.
SYNOPSIS. Vocal repertoires of male and female non-human primatescan differ by presenceor absence of sounds, acoustic form, andfunction or usage. Examples of sex-specificity in vocalizationsare taken from field studies of two macaque species that differin social organizationand habitat. They are used to illustratesex-specific acoustic patterns and vocalizations thatare employedin socially different fashion by males and females. Some vocalsex differences develop from sounds shared in common by younganimals of both sexes while others arise late in life by anacoustic pattern suddenly emerging only in males. Differencesmay be due to sexual selection, social suppression, morphologicalfeatures, or because vocal sounds are linked to circumstancesexperienced by only male or female adults. The relation betweensocial usage of some vocalizations by adults and youngstersaffords insights into their signal value and into the underlyingstate of the vocalizing animal. 相似文献
15.
The purpose of this study was to investigate the manipulative propensities of a captive group of lion-tailed macaques (Macaca silenus). Simple natural objects (browse and bamboo poles) were provided regularly in the home cage. Findings indicate richness in the frequency and form of manipulative activities, with juvenile males manipulating the test objects more frequently and exhibiting more goal-directed manipulative activity than adult females. A variety of goal-directed manipulative activities (use of objects to act as ladders, to apply leverage, and to create perches) occurred spontaneously, with some instances involving joint action or social use. These data are consistent with the hypotheses that macaques possess extensive capacities for object exploration and social facilitation, and that an evolutionary history of omnivorous foraging habits correlates positively with the expression of anomalous sensorimotor skills. 相似文献
16.
Biswas S Kalanidhi AP Ashok MS Reddy GS Srinivasan VA Rangarajan PN 《Indian journal of experimental biology》2001,39(6):533-536
A rabies DNA vaccine consisting of plasmid DNA expressing the rabies virus surface glycoprotein was injected (im) twice at two week interval to outbred swiss mice or Bonnet monkeys (Macaca radiata) and the levels of rabies virus neutralizing antibody (VNA) titres were examined over a one year period. In mice, the VNA titre was maintained above the minimum protective level (0.5 I.U./ml) up to 10 months after primary immunization, while in monkeys, the titre dropped below the protective level by 6 months. An anamnestic B cell response was seen in both mice and monkeys following the administration of a booster dose, 10 and 6 months after the primary immunization, respectively. These results indicate that im injection of rabies DNA vaccine induces VNA in nonhuman primates and mice unlike intradermal (id) immunization, which was shown to induce VNA only in mice but not in monkeys. This is the first report on the induction of VNA in nonhuman primates by im inoculation of rabies DNA vaccine. 相似文献
17.
G. Hohmann 《Primates; journal of primatology》1989,30(3):325-345
Field observations and spectrographic analyses of sound recordings of South Indian bonnet macaques revealed a vocal repertoire
of at least 25 basic patterns. The repertoire consists of well separated sound classes and acoustic categories connected by
structural intergradation. Besides structural variations within and between different elements of the repertoire, the vocal
system ofMacaca radiata is characterized by regular combinations of particular basic patterns. These combinations occurred not only between calls
of similar structure and function but also between calls usually emitted in entirely different social contexts. According
to the qualitative analysis, sex-specific asymmetries of the vocal behaviour were less pronounced than age-dependent characteristics.
The comparison of clear call vocalizations ofMacaca radiata andM. fuscata revealed significant species-specific differences on the structural and the behavioural level. Evaluations of the structural
features of alarm calls of various macaque species imply marked differences between members of thefascicularis group andsinica group on one hand and thesilenus group andarctoides group on the other. 相似文献
18.
Infant development in lion-tailed monkeys was observed during the first eight weeks after birth. A rapid decrease in the time
spent in the closest association by the mother and the infant was seen between the first and second weeks. Between the second
and fifth week there was a somewhat steady state with only marginal decrease in the time spent by the infant out of lap of
the mother. However, during this time, a rapid decline in maternal cradling and restraining behaviour was seen. From the sixth
week onwards, there was a rapid increase in the time spent by the infant out of contact with the mother. Independence of the
infant during troop progression was seen only towards the eighth week, that too only marginal. Among the troop members the
subadult females and the juvenile females showed the maximum interest in the infant. The former spent much more time than
the juveniles interacting with the infant. The adult females showed only little interest in the infant while the adult male
showed no interest at all. Infant snatching by subadult females and juvenile females was seen on many occasions. The infant
remained in the possession of them from less than 1 min to more than 2 hr. This behaviour tended to decrease towards the sixth
week. 相似文献
19.
Yukimaru Sugiyama 《Primates; journal of primatology》1971,12(3-4):247-266
The social behavior of wild bonnet macaques impresses upon us a peculiality and uniqueness which is different from other macaques. Although there is a clear-cut ranking relationship among adult males and they carry out their proper social roles according to each situation, many social behaviors are carried out independently of or ignoring the ranking order. The animals are organized in a flexible and tolerant social unit, but they don't abandon the advantages of group life.The uniqueness of bonnet behavior and society is recognized to be related to the highly developed individuality of this species. 相似文献