首页 | 官方网站   微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Agrell  J. 《Behavioral ecology》1995,6(2):182-191
Home range size, overlap between home ranges, movement patternsand social behavior in two generations of female field voleswere examined by radio tracking unrelated individuals in enclosedareas. In addition, potential effects of increased relatednesswere examined by comparing setups with groups of sisters andunrelated females. Overwintered females showed less overlapbetween ranges and moved shorter and more irregular distancescompared with females of the year. Based on responses to introduced,unfamiliar conspecifics it was concluded that the overdisperseddistribution of overwintered females was maintained primarilyby avoidance behavior and that infanticidal behavior was equallyfrequent in the two generations. Observed differences betweengenerations could not be ascribed to differences in relatedness,and experimental increase of the degree of relatedness had negligibleeffects. The overdispersion and restricted movements shown byoverwintered females are instead suggested to be caused by acombination of low food availability and exposure to intensepredation in spring/early summer, whereas spacing and movementpatterns of females of the year are less influenced by thesefactors. As aggressive interactions are rare and populationdensity is usually low in spring, it is unlikely that breedingdensity and population growth is restrained by the social behaviorof overwintered females.  相似文献   

2.
Bright colors in birds might signal that they are undesirableas prey (aposematic), an idea that has been difficult to test.When stuffed pied flycatchers Ficedula hypoleuca are exposedto migrating sparrowhawks Accipiter nisus in spring or in autumn,the hawks attack cryptic females more often than bright males.To achieve better statistical control and to assess whethermale plumage also reduces predation risk in the breeding seasonand in the nesting habitat of the pied flycatcher, I placedpairs of male and female flycatcher mounts in similar positionsnear 22 nests of sparrowhawks. The hawks attacked mainly femalemounts, verifying that the preference is real. The sparrowhawkscaught at least 19 live pied flycatchers; 12 young, 5 adultmales, 1 adult female, and 1 female or young. Hawks that caughtan adult male seemed to prefer attacking female mounts. I discussthree interpretations of these results, suggesting that black-and-whitemale flycatchers may benefit from being a novel and aberrantprey, at least early in the breeding season.  相似文献   

3.
We examined the effects of predation risk on the behavior ofrhinoceros auklets (Cerorhinca monocerata) breeding at PineIsland, British Columbia, in 1990. Provisioning parents in someareas of the colony risked predation by bald eagles (Haliacetusleucocephalus). Chicks in high and low predation risk areasof the colony hatched on approximately the same date, receivedsimilar amounts of food to 46 days of age, grew at the samerate, reached similar peak masses, and fledged at similar masses.However, chicks in high predation areas fledged at a youngerage than did chicks in low predation areas. These data are consistentwith the hypothesis that parents in high risk areas terminatedprovisioning several days before those in lower risk areas.Mass at fledging was inversely related to age at fledging inboth high and low risk areas. The regression line for the highrisk habitats lies below that from the low risk habitats, aspredicted by a model that examines optimal time of fledgingfrom the perspective of the parents. We conclude that risk ofpredation represents a significant cost of reproduction to somerhinoceros auklets and that individual auklets within the colonyvary their behavior according to predation risk.  相似文献   

4.
Predicting group size in primates: foraging costs and predation risks   总被引:17,自引:2,他引:17  
We present a direct test of the long-standing hypothesis thatfood competition limits primate group size. Group size is acritical social variable because it constrains most other aspectsof social organization. We develop a simple population-specificindex of indirect feeding competition based on daily foragingcosts. This index explains nearly two-thirds of between-populationvariation in mean group sizes of mostly fruit-eating (but notof mostly leaf-eating) primates. Group size is also significantlyrelated to body size and terrestriality (or use of open country),which are suspected correlates of predation risk, although feedingcompetition remains an important predictor of group size evenwhen these correlates are controlled. Phylogeny also appearsto be important: the differences between observed mean populationgroup sizes and those predicted using ecological factors aremost positive for the Old World monkeys and most negative forthe lemuroids in our sample. The weak relationship between groupsize and feeding competition found for folivorous species maybe explained either by the energetic constraints of a leafydiet or by limits to group size imposed by infanticide as ahabitual male reproductive strategy.  相似文献   

5.
The definition of eusociality   总被引:7,自引:2,他引:5  
We describe more precise definitions for the term "eusociality"and other social systems. Our criterion for eusociality is thepresence of castes, which are groups of individuals that becomeirreversibly behaviorally distinct at some point prior to reproductivematurity. Eusocial societies are characterized by two traits:(1) helping by individuals of the less-reproductive caste, and(2) either behavioral totipotency of only the more reproductivecaste (facultative eusociality) or totipotency of neither caste(obligate eusociality). We define "cooperative breeding" asalloparental care without castes. Cooperatively breeding societiesmay comprise two types, semisocial (distribution of lifetimereproductive success bimodal), and quasisocial (distributionof lifetime reproductive success unimodal), but this hypothesisrequires empirical analysis. Our definitions conceptually unifystudies of arthropod and vertebrate sociality.  相似文献   

6.
The immunocompetence hypothesis predicts that testosterone (T)enhances the expression of male secondary sexual characterswhile exerting a suppressive effect on the immune system therebyexposing hosts to higher intensities of parasite infestations.In a natural population of barn swallow (Hirundo rustica) males,the intensity of infestation by some ectoparasites was negativelycorrelated with tail length and was positively correlated withimmunoglobulin levels, but no clear relationship was observedbetween immune responses (leukocyte counts, immunoglobulins)and tail length. Males implanted with T had higher intensitiesof parasite infestations at the time of recapture than controlmales, and T-implanted males experienced an increase in countsof eosinophils. In T-implanted males, immunoglobulin levelsinitially decreased and then increased as time from implantationelapsed. Among T-implanted males, those with longer tails hada smaller increase in eosinophil counts, tended to experiencea smaller increase of parasite infestations, and were more likelyto survive until the following breeding season than those withshorter tails. The relationships between parasite burden, immunesystem, and exaggeration of tail length in the natural populationof males are consistent with some aspects of the immunocompetencehandicap hypothesis. The results from the manipulation of Tplasma levels are also partly consistent with the hypothesis,since T-implantation resulted in higher levels of parasite infestations,but contradict the assumption of an obligatory immunosuppressiveeffect of T. Higher activation of the immune system of T-implantedmales indicate that high T plasma levels imposed a two-foldcost because of the effects on parasites and the immune responseto parasites, and this suggests that the effect of T on parasitesmight not be mediated by the immune system of the host. Theresults of the manipulation of T plasma levels support the handicapversion of the immunocompetence hypothesis since high quality,long-tailed males paid less in terms of activation of the immunesystem, change in parasite infestations, and chances of survivalthan low-quality, short-tailed males.  相似文献   

7.
Although females of numerous species possess genetically-basedpreferences for certain male trails and male preferentiallywith males possessing these traits, recent theoretical and experimentalevidence indicates that they may also copy (imitate) the matechoices of other females under certain circumstances. Such mate-choicecopying is expected to be most prevalent when females have theopportunity to observe the mate choices of others and when matechoice is costly to females. One potential direct fitness costof mate choice is increased individual risk of mortality dueto predation. Here, we investigate for the first time the effectof increasing the apparent risk of predation on the tendencyof females to copy the experimentally staged mate choice ofanother female. Using adult female guppies (Poecilia reticulata)originating from a Trinidadian population that experiences arelatively high fish predation pressure, we first establishthat they possess a preference for the more brighdy coloredof two simultaneously presented males in the absence of bothmate-copying opportunity and an immediate threat of predation.However, most females reversed their initial mate preferencewhen given an opportunity to copy the mate preference of anotherfemale in the absence of predation threat The proportion offemales reversing their preference when given the opportunityto do so was not affected by increasing the apparent risk of(fish) predation. This result may be owing either to femaleguppies tending to copy the mate choice of others whenever theopportunity arises because the benefits of doing so accrue irrespectiveof the ambient risk of predation or to females choosing randomlybetween males with respect to their color pattern in the presenceof the predator irrespective of mate-copying opportunity. Thesetwo explanations for the apparent lack of an effect of predationrisk on mate-choice copying per se are both plausible but unfortunatelycould not be easily distinguished here. It may thus be possible,and interesting, that individual female guppies chose randomlybetween the available males in the presence of the predatorbut otherwise copied the choice of others when given the opportunityto do so.  相似文献   

8.
Sex role reversal in birds is usually associated with paternalcare of both eggs and chicks. This pattern of care typicallyleads to the potential rate of reproduction of males being lowerthan that of females. Hence, operational sex-ratio theory predictsthat each male should be under strong selection to avoid beingcuckolded. A male should, therefore, guard his female partner(s)from extrapair copulation attempts by other males. Furthermore,the sexual conflict theory of copulation behavior predicts thatin species with extensive paternal care the male should controlthe temporal pattern of copulations—copulations shouldoccur both frequently and throughout the prelaying period. Wetested these predictions in the Eurasian dotterel (Charadriusmorinsllus), in which the male usually provides all the parentalcare. In accordance with the first prediction, male dotterelsdid "guard" their pair-female prior to egg-laying. Contraryto the second prediction, however, copulations were not frequentand did not occur throughout the pre-laying phase-despite frequentsolicitation by the female, copulations only occurred immediatelyprior to egg-laying. Nevertheless, male-initiated courtshipwas both coincident with the pattern of copulations and morelikely than female-initiated courtship to result in copulation.Our results do, therefore, appear to agree with the centralprediction of the sexual conflict theory that males should controlthe pattern of copulations. We suggest that male dotterels willcopulate only after several days of being paired because theyface a duel risk of cuckoldry from both extrapair copulationand rapid mate switching. We tested the realized incidence ofcuckoldry using DNA fingerprinting. Only 4.6% (2/44) of chickswere not the genetic offspring of the caring male correspondingto 9.1% (2/22) broods affected. The rate of extrapair paternityin the dotterel is, therefore, relatively low compared to thatin many other avian species. We conclude that male dotterelssuccessfully protect their paternity of the brood for whichthey care through a combined strategy of mate guarding and strategictiming of copulations.  相似文献   

9.
Through computer simulations, we model three different foodfinding strategies: searcher, no information transfer, watcher,limited information transfer; follower, full information transfer.The aim of this article was to study how frequency-dependentselection affects the proportion of these strategies at a simulatedcolony under different patterns of food distribution. Furthermore,we determined how information transfer in a population witha mixed evolutionarily stable strategy (ESS) modified the averageforaging efficiency of an individual compared to that of anindividual in a population with mutual information exchange.We found that the proportion of information gaining strategiesincreases as the food resources become more clumped. The improvementin foraging efficiency through the operation of an informationcenter need not require mutuality in information exchange. Onthe basis of the presented study, at the ESS only a small percentageof colony members need discover food patches, yet the foragingefficiency may be high because of the operation of an informationcenter.  相似文献   

10.
Simmons  L. W. 《Behavioral ecology》1995,6(4):376-381
Sexual selection in field crickets (Gryllidae) is well documented.Several studies have identified male traits, such as age andbody size, that influence pairing success in the field. HereI show how these traits covary with male quality by examiningthe degree of fluctuating asymmetry in male Gryllus campestris.Older males were both larger and more symmetrical. Principalcomponents analysis suggests that these three variables wereessentially measures of the same trait, male quality. A comparisonof paired and calling males showed that males of high qualitywere more successful in obtaining mates; paired males were older,larger, and more symmetrical. The area of the harp covariedwith morphological traits and determined the carrier frequencyof the male's call. Information related to male quality wastherefore available for female discrimination.  相似文献   

11.
Energetic constraints and foraging efficiency   总被引:2,自引:1,他引:1  
Previous research considers foraging options that differ interms of their gross rate of gain b and rate of energy expenditurec. This research argues that maximizing efficiency b/c willmaximize net energetic gain when there is an upper limit onthe amount of energy that can be assimilated. This analysisdoes not include the expenditure during the time for which theanimal is unable to forage because of this constraint. Whenthis expenditure is included, maximizing efficiency is no longeroptimal. Instead the best feeding option is the one with thehighest value of b/(cc1), where c, is the metabolicrate when the animal is not foraging.  相似文献   

12.
Sexual selection theory predicts that the larger sex shouldbe that for which fitness increases at the faster rate withsize. In butterflies, as in most invertebrates, females areusually the larger sex, but previous comparative analysis hasshown that relative male size increases with female polyandryamong butterflies. In agreement with this pattern, males arelarger than females in the strongly polyandrous green-veinedwhite butterfly, Pieris napi L., and in this article we assessthe size dependence of reproductive success in both sexes. Inan experiment where virgin males and females were released inthe field, we found no strong association between size and malemating success. However, laboratory experiments showed thatthere was a strong correlation between size and the ejaculatethat the male delivered to the female at mating and that largeejaculates delayed female remating for a longer time comparedto small ejaculates. Moreover, female P. napi utilize male-derivednutrients received at mating to increase their fecundity. Hence,large males sire more offspring both by way of donating morenutrients to female egg production and by way of delaying femaleremating (given that the last male to mate with the female willfather most of the offspring). Laboratory experiments showedthat the association between size and fecundity was low, ornonexistent, among P. napi females allowed to mate only once.However, weak size dependence was found for polyandrous females.We hypothesize that size dependence of female fecundity maybe especially weak among polyandrous butterflies because a fundamentalsource of variation in fecundity relates to their ability tofind nutrient giving males, an ability which may be unrelatedto female size. According to this hypothesis there is a causalassociation between weak size dependence of female fecundityand polyandry, and a strong size dependence of male reproductivesuccess that may underlie the comparative pattern of positivecorrelation between relative male size and polyandry.  相似文献   

13.
This study deals with dispersal behavior of sexuals and intraspecificvariation in queen numbers. The specific questions are: (1)Is there an association between male and female dispersal behaviorand the number of queens in a colony? (2) Is there an associationbetween individual behavior and physiological condition? (3)Do males and females from monogyne (one queen per colony) andpolygyne (several functional queens per colony) colonies differwith respect to size, weight, and physiological condition? Theresults show that both males and females are more prone to dispersein monogyne than in polygyne colonies. Moreover, males and femalesof both monogyne and polygyne colonies show dispersal polymorphism,suggesting that an increased tendency of reproductive femalesto stay in the maternal colony may cause monogyne colonies toswitch to polygyny. The propensity to disperse is associatedwith the physiological condition of individuals. Larger andheavier females containing more fat and glycogen preferentiallydisperse, whereas smaller ones with less fat and glycogen moreeasily dealate and mate without a previous nuptial flight. Maledispersal correlates positively to larger size and higher levelsof glycogen; fat contents do not increase during maturation.The females produced in monogyne colonies are larger, heavier,and contain more fat and glycogen than those produced in polygynecolonies. The males produced in monogyne colonies have relativelylonger wings and are heavier than those produced in polygynecolonies. However, there are no differences in size and fatcontents between males from monogyne and polygyne colonies.  相似文献   

14.
We studied the effect of operational sex ratio on female reluctanceand male persistence to mate as well as on the length of copulationand postcopulatory guarding in Gerris lacustris by adding fivesurplus males or females to the basin with a pair in tandem.In the control treatment, a pair alone was tested. Accordingto the copulatory guarding hypothesis (CGH), males should prolongmating and guard females in the presence of surplus males. Accordingto the convenience polyandry hypothesis (CPH), females shouldshow lower levels of resistance to prolonged mating in the presenceof surplus males because the mating male protects the femaleagainst harassment from other males. As expected on the basisof both the CGH and CPH, mating (copulation + guarding) averagedlonger in the male-biased treatment. The behavior of males andfemales during mating suggested that both hypotheses hold true:females showed less resistance to prolonged mating (as predictedfrom CPH), and male behavior suggested stronger efforts to stayon the female when surplus males were present (as predictedfrom CGH). Comparisons of the treatment with surplus femaleswith the results from the mating pair without surplus individualssuggested that the capabilities of water striders in tandemto assess the sex of nearby nonmating striders are limited.  相似文献   

15.
Two major theories have been developed to explain the locationand evolution of leks. According to the hot-spot model leksare male initiated and occur because males aggregate at locationsof maximum female home range overlap. The alternative femalepreference model, on the other hand, assumes that leks are femaleinitiated, and that females prefer to mate with males that aggregate.Small heath butterfly leks are situated close to landmarks suchas trees and bushes in open grassland. Censuses of wild populationsand experiments with artificial landmarks showed that theirattractiveness increased with height and width measured at themiddle of tree height. Landmark width at field layer heightwas negatively correlated with attractiveness, which suggeststhat a cornet shape is preferred to a pyramid shape. Togetherthese landmark size variables explained 60% of variation inlek size. Release experiments showed that landmarks were approachedby receptive virgin females, but mated females were indifferentto them. Landmark use was correlated to microclimatic conditions.Males preferred wider landmarks during cooler weather. Theyalso competed for the lee side of landmarks. There was no correlationbetween the location of landmarks and the dispersion of matedfemales or emergence sites of females. Hence, receptive femaledispersion was largely determined by their mate-locating behavior,which in turn appeared to result from male dispersion. Leksmay initially have developed around easily detected visual cuesthat offered a more suitable microclimate for male activity.Females visiting these locations would find a suitable matemore rapidly and save valuable time for oviposition. Althoughnonadaptive evolution cannot be excluded, taken together theresults support the female preference model for lek evolution.  相似文献   

16.
The decision by fishermen to discard or retain fish of low valueto make room for more valuable fish in the hold of a boat (high-grading)is similar to diet choice problems faced by natural foragers.In our study, we apply the rationale of diet choice theory tohigh-grading behavior in the Oregon trawl fishery by treatingfishermen as foragers who must decide how much of each net'shaul to "ingest" before searching for more prey. We derive astate-dependent, temporal model of discarding behavior withina fishing trip. This optimization considers the availabilityof differently valued fish, trip quotas set by the regulatoryagency, and the risk of premature trip termination due to lossof gear or injury. The results indicate that those parametersaffect discarding behavior through their effect on the probabilityof exceeding the allowable catch, which we consider analogousto gut capacity. High-grading (partial prey consumption) occurredthroughout many simulated trips. The predictions were consistentwith the trends in discarding observed in the Oregon trawl fleet.Behavioral models such as ours can be useful to fishery managersby providing a means to explore the potential responses of fishermento new regulations before they are implemented.  相似文献   

17.
Males of the black wheatear (Oenanthe leucura) carry on averagealmost 2 kg of stones to cavities inside caves before the layingof each clutch, a display involved in postmating sexual selectionas determined from female adjustment of timing and rate of reproductionto experimentally manipulated numbers of stones carried. A largewing area in relation to body mass would allow males to carrymany and heavy stones. The number and mass of stones carriedwere inversely related to wing loading, suggesting that a largewing area has evolved as an adaptation to stone carrying. Wetested this functional hypothesis in a field experiment withthree treatments: (1) two primaries removed from each wing (manipulation),(2) the tips of two primaries removed from each wing (sham-manipulation),and (3) the male just captured and handled (control). The numberand mass of stones carried were inversely related to originalwing area, as predicted by the hypothesis, and males with initiallylarge wing areas were better able to cope with the experimentaltreatment than others. These results are consistent with stonecarrying being a reliable signal of maximum working abilityof males during short bursts of stone carrying, and intraspecificdifferences in wing morphology therefore allow males to displayat different levels.  相似文献   

18.
We studied the nest defense behavior of Eurasian kestrels (Falcotinnunculus) towards a stuffed pine marten (Martes martes) througha 3-year vole cycle (1990–92) in western Finland. Survivalprobability of offspring decreases with a later start of breeding,and, therefore, early breeders should protect their offspringmore than late ones. We found this true for males during theincubation period, but not for females. In addition, we expectedthe nest defense intensity to increase with offspring number.During the incubation period, this was true for females, butnot for males. During the nestling phase, parents did not adjusttheir defense effort to natural or manipulated (by one to twoyoung) brood size. Survival prospects of kestrel offspring werehighest in the increasing vole year 1991 and lowest in the decreasingvole year 1992, and, therefore, we expected the defense activityof kestrels to follow the same trend. However, the oppositeresult appeared true for females with a similar tendency formales. Most hypotheses predicting avian nest defense behaviorwere not supported by our data. Temporally heterogeneous environmentand low degree of nest-site tenacity of migratory kestrels maymake them unfamiliar with environmental variation and survivalprospects of their offspring. Therefore, fitness benefits ofparental care are not predictable, and kestrels may thus adjusttheir parental effort to their own future reproductive potential(i.e., number of future breeding attempts), rather than to somecurrent investment indicator, like offspring age and number.  相似文献   

19.
Many social behaviors are conditional, but behavioral comparisonsbetween populations do not normally distinguish genetic andenvironmental causation. As a result, the opportunity to testpredictions about the evolution of strategic conditionality(genotype x environment interaction) is lost. We apply theseconcepts in an examination of how interpopulation differencesin mean and variance of sex ratio have led to genetic differencesin the allocation of male effort to mate guarding versus nonguardingbetween genetically isolated populations of the soapberry bugin Oklahoma and Florida. We observed the mating behavior ofmales from the two populations at a series of experimental sexratios, and modeled their mating decisions as first-order Markovchains of independent mating states. Likelihood ratio testsof these behavioral sequences showed that the populations differedsignificantly in their response to sex ratio, and that onlymales from the variable environment (Oklahoma) altered theirbehavior in response to differences in female availability amongthe treatments. The flexible strategy of this population maybe adaptive and probably has evolved in response to sex ratiovariability.  相似文献   

20.
Sexual selection may be reduced by costs of mate choice suchas predation risk and energetic expense. Despite their theoreticalimportance, such costs have rarely been quantified or relatedto the quality of mate obtained. We performed such a study witha wild population of redlip blennies in Barbados. In this coralreef fish, females traveled 0.2–12 m from their feedingterritories to the territories of nesting males, where theyspawned and left their eggs in the males' care. Previous studieshave shown that larger and older males are better parents andare preferred by females. Here, we found that long-distancetrips by females were made primarily toward larger males, andthe extra distance traveled beyond the nearest male was correlatedwith the improvement in size of male obtained. Comparisons withfeeding budgets while the females remained on their home territoriessuggested that costs from lost time and energy were minor, andno predation attempts on females were observed. However, onlonger trips the females faced more aggressive attacks fromdamselfishes, whose territories they crossed. In contrast, noattacks were observed from damselfish when the blennies remainedon their own territories. These attacks appeared to be responsiblefor a greater incidence of scars on females than on males. Thisdifference between the sexes became more pronounced as spawningprogressed, and then decreased as females healed during thenonspawning interval. Thus, female blennies endure a cost ofinterspecific harassment which is correlated with the qualityof mate obtained. This cost may restrain sexual selection inthis species.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司    京ICP备09084417号-23

京公网安备 11010802026262号